Syllable
A. Definition of Syllable
The syllable is a well-recognized unit
in linguistic analysis which explains quite well the number of rhythmic units
that will be perceived in a word or longer utterance. This number is usually
equal to the number of vowels in the utterance. Although it is usually easy to
get agreement on the number of syllables present in a word, intuitions
sometimes differ over where the boundaries between one syllable and another
should be placed. Despite such hesitation, the syllable has proven to be a very
useful concept in discussing the general rules for distribution of sounds in
languages.
Where listeners differ in syllabifying
particular words, it is generally the case that both possible syllabifications
can be shown to be permitted ones since unambiguous cases of each type can be
found. For example, an English word such as pastry might be
syllabified by different speakers as past.ry or as pas.try (where
the dot represents a division between syllables). Since both paste and tree
are perfectly acceptable monosyllabic words of English, either division will agree with a broader
rule concerning possible syllables of English.
The broadest rules of this kind for any given
language describe what is called the canonical syllable pattern of the
language. This is the pattern which essentially characterizes how many
consonants may occur before the vowel in a syllable, and how many after the
vowel. A syllable is a unit of organization for a sequence of speech sounds. According to David Chrystal
in his book A Dictionary of Linguistics
and Phonetics
syllable (n.) (syll) A unit of pronunciation
typically larger than a single sound and smaller than a word. A word may be
pronounced ‘syllable at a time’, as in ne-ver-the-less, and a good
dictionary will indicate where these syllabic divisions occur in
writing, thus providing information about how a word may be hyphenated. The
notion of syllable, in short, is very real to native-speakers, and is often
used in a quasi-technical sense in everyday conversation (e.g. Shall I put
it in words of one syllable?).
Based on Merriam Webster dictionary syllable are ;
a unit of
spoken language that is next bigger than a speech sound and consists of one or
more vowel sounds alone or of a syllabic consonant alone or of either with one or more
consonant sounds preceding or following
one or more letters (as syl, la, and ble)
in a word (as syl*la*ble) usually set off from the rest of the word by a
centered dot or a hyphen and roughly corresponding to the syllables of spoken language and treated as helps to
pronunciation or as guides to placing hyphens at the end of a line .
A syllable is a unit of pronunciation uttered without interruption, loosely, a
single sound. All words are made from at least one syllable. Monosyllables
have only one vowel sound; polysyllables have more than one. If a
syllable ends with a consonant, it is called a closed syllable. If a
syllable ends with a vowel, it is called an open syllable. Patterns of
syllables can be shown with C and V (C for 'consonant', V for 'vowel'). Closed
syllables are shown as CVC, open syllables CV. Some languages
like English have many kinds of closed syllables. Some
languages like Japanese have few kinds of closed syllables.
B. Kind of Syllable
There
are six different kinds of syllables in English:
- Closed
Syllables: A closed syllable has one and only one vowel,
and it ends in a consonant.
Examples include in, ask, truck, sock,
stretch, twelfth, and on.
- Open
Syllables: An open syllable has one and only one vowel,
and that vowel occurs at the end of the syllable.
Examples include no, she, I, a,
and spry.
- Silent-E
Syllables: A silent-e syllable ends in an e, has
one and only one consonant before that e, and has one and only one
vowel before that consonant.
Examples include ate, ice, tune, slope,
strobe, and these.
- Vowel
Combination Syllables: A vowel combination syllable
has a cluster of two or three vowels or a vowel-consonant unit with a
sound or sounds particular to that unit.
Examples include rain, day, see, veil,
pie, piece, noise, toy, cue, and true.
- Vowel-R
Syllables: A vowel-r syllable is one which includes one
and only one vowel followed by an r, or one vowel followed by an r
which is followed by a silent e, or a vowel
combination followed by an r.
Examples include car, or, care, ire,
air, and deer.
- Consonant-L-E
Syllables: In these syllables, a consonant is followed by le.
The vowel sound in these syllables is the schwa sound that occurs before
the l.
Examples
include -ble, -cle, -dle, -fle, and -gle.
C. Syllable structure
Three
representation of a syllable. In most theories of phonology, the general
structure of a syllable (σ) consists of three segments:
consonant, obligatory in some languages, optional or
even restricted in others
sonant, obligatory in most languages
consonant, optional in some languages, highly
restricted or prohibited in others
The syllable is usually considered right-branching,
i.e. nucleus and coda are grouped together as a "rime" and are only
distinguished at the second level. However, in some traditional descriptions of
certain languages, the syllable is considered left-branching, i.e. onset and
nucleus group below a higher-level unit, called a "body" or
"core":
right branch, contrasts with onset, splits into
nucleus and coda
left branch, contrasts with coda, splits into onset
and nucleus
In some theories the onset is strictly consonantal,
thus necessitating another segment before the nucleus:
often termed onset, but leaving out semi-vowels
glide between initial, if any, and nucleus or rime
contrasts with initial, extended rime
Although every syllable has supra-segmental features,
these are usually ignored if not semantically relevant, e.g. in tonal languages.
may be carried by the syllable as a whole or by the rime
In some theories of phonology, these syllable
structures are displayed as tree diagrams (similar to the trees found in some types of syntax).
Not all phonologists agree that syllables have internal structure; in fact,
some phonologists doubt the existence of the syllable as a theoretical entity.[2]
The nucleus is usually the vowel in the middle
of a syllable. The onset is the sound or sounds occurring before the
nucleus, and the coda (literally 'tail') is the sound or sounds that
follow the nucleus. They are sometimes collectively known as the shell.
The term rime covers the nucleus plus coda. In the one-syllable English
word cat, the nucleus is a (the sound that can be shouted or sung
on its own), the onset c, the coda t, and the rime at.
This syllable can be abstracted as a consonant-vowel-consonant syllable,
abbreviated CVC. Languages vary greatly in the restrictions on the
sounds making up the onset, nucleus and coda of a syllable, according to what
is termed a language's phonotactics.
·
Onset
Most syllables have an onset. Some languages restrict
onsets to be only a single consonant, while others allow multiconsonant onsets
according to various rules. For example, in English, onsets such as pr-,
pl- and tr- are possible but tl- is not (except very
marginally in foreign words such as Tlingit), and sk- is possible but ks- is not.
In Greek,
however, both ks- and tl- are possible onsets, while contrarily
in Classical Arabic no multiconsonant onsets are allowed at all.
Some languages require all syllables to have an onset;
in these languages a null onset such as in the English word
"at" is not possible. This is less strange than it may appear at
first, as most such languages allow syllables to begin with a phonemic glottal stop (the sound in the middle of English
"uh-oh"). Furthermore, in English and most other languages, a word
that begins with a vowel is automatically pronounced with an initial glottal
stop when following a pause, whether or not a glottal stop occurs as a phoneme
in the language.
Consequently,
few languages make a phonemic distinction between a word beginning with a vowel
and a word beginning with a glottal stop followed by a vowel, since the
distinction will generally only be audible following another word. (However, Hawaiian
and a number of other Polynesian languages do make such a distinction; cf. Hawaiian /ahi/
"fire", /ʔahi/ "tuna".)
This means that the difference between a syllable with
a null onset and one beginning with a glottal stop is often purely a difference
of phonological
analysis, rather than the actual pronunciation of the syllable. In some cases,
the pronunciation of a (putatively) vowel-initial word when following another
word – particularly, whether or not a glottal stop is inserted – indicates
whether the word should be considered to have a null onset. For example, many Romance languages such as Spanish never insert such a glottal stop, while English
does so only some of the time, depending on factors such as conversation speed;
in both cases, this suggests that the words in question are truly
vowel-initial.
But there are
exceptions here, too. For example, German and Arabic both require that a glottal stop be inserted between
a word and a following, putatively vowel-initial word. Yet such words are said
to begin with a vowel in German but a glottal stop in Arabic. The reason for
this has to do with other properties of the two languages. For example, a
glottal stop does not occur in other situations in German, e.g. before a
consonant or at the end of word. On the other hand, in Arabic, not only does a
glottal stop occur in such situations (e.g. Classical /saʔala/
"he asked", /raʔj/ "opinion", /dˤawʔ/
"light"), but it occurs in alternations that are clearly indicative
of its phonemic status (cf. Classical /kaːtib/ "writer" vs. /maktuːb/
"written", /ʔaːkil/ "eater" vs. /maʔkuːl/
"eaten").
The writing system of a language may not correspond
with the phonological analysis of the language in terms of its handling of
(potentially) null onsets. For example, in some languages written in the Latin alphabet, an initial glottal stop is left unwritten; on the
other hand, some languages written using non-Latin alphabets such as abjads and abugidas have a special zero
consonant to represent a null
onset.
As an example,
in Hangul, the alphabet of the Korean
language, a null onset is
represented with ㅇ at the left or top section of a graph, as in 역
"station", pronounced yeok, where the diphthong yeo is the nucleus and k is the coda.
·
Nucleus
Generally, every syllable requires a nucleus
(sometimes called the peak), and the minimal syllable consists only of a
nucleus, as in the English words "eye" or "owe". The
syllable nucleus is usually a vowel, in the form of a monophthong, diphthong, or triphthong, but sometimes is a syllabic consonant. By far the most common syllabic consonants are sonorants like [l], [r], [m], [n] or [ŋ], but a few languages have so-called syllabic fricatives, also known as fricative vowels. (In the
context of Chinese
phonology, the related but
non-synonymous term apical vowel is commonly used.) Mandarin Chinese is famous for having such sounds in at least some of
its dialects, for example the pinyin syllables sī shī rī, sometimes pronounced [sź̩ ʂʐ̩́ ʐʐ̩́]
respectively.
A few languages, such as Nuxalk (Bella Coola), even allow stop
consonants and voiceless fricatives as syllabic nuclei. However, linguists have analyzed
this situation in various ways, some arguing that such syllables have no
nucleus at all, and some arguing that the concept of "syllable"
cannot clearly be applied at all to these languages. See the discussion below
concerning syllable-less
languages.
·
Coda
A coda-less syllable of the form V, CV, CCV, etc. is
called an open syllable (or free syllable), while a syllable that
has a coda (VC, CVC, CVCC, etc.) is called a closed syllable (or checked
syllable). Note that they have nothing to do with open and close vowels. Almost all languages allow open syllables, but some,
such as Hawaiian, do not have closed syllables.
In English, consonants have been analyzed as acting
simultaneously as the coda of one syllable and the onset of the following
syllable, as in 'bellow' bel-low, a phenomenon known as ambisyllabicity.
It is argued that words such as arrow /ˈæroʊ/
can't be divided into separately pronounceable syllables: neither /æ/ nor
/ær/ is
a possible independent syllable, and likewise with the other short vowels /ɛ ɪ ɒ ʌ ʊ/.
However, Wells (1990) argues against ambisyllabicity in English, positing that
consonants and consonant clusters are codas when after a stressed syllable
followed by an unstressed syllable, or after a full vowel and followed by a
reduced syllable, and are onsets in other contexts.
Rhyme
·
Medial
and final
In the phonology of some East Asian languages,
especially Chinese, the syllable structure is expanded to include an
additional, optional segment known as a medial, which is located between
the onset (often termed the initial in this context) and the rime. The
medial is normally a glide
consonant, but reconstructions
of Old Chinese
generally include liquid medials (/r/ in modern reconstructions, /l/ in
older versions), and many reconstructions of Middle
Chinese include a medial
contrast between /i/ and /j/, where the /i/ functions phonologically as a glide rather than as
part of the nucleus.
In addition,
many reconstructions of both Old and Middle Chinese include complex medials
such as /rj/, /ji/, /jw/ and /jwi/. The medial groups phonologically with the rime
rather than the onset, and the combination of medial and rime is collectively
known as the final.
·
Tone
In most languages, the pitch or pitch contour in which
a syllable is pronounced conveys shades of meaning such as emphasis or
surprise, or distinguishes a statement from a question. In tonal languages, however, the pitch of a word affects the basic
lexical meaning (e.g "cat" vs. "dog") or grammatical
meaning (e.g. past vs. present). In some languages, only the pitch itself (e.g.
high vs. low) has this effect, while in others, especially East Asian languages
such as Chinese, Thai or Vietnamese, the shape or contour (e.g. level vs. rising vs.
falling) also needs to be distinguished
References
Crystal,
David, 1985, A Dictionary of Linguistic
and Phonetics, Oxford: Basil Blackwell
Booij,
Geert, 2007, The Grammar of Words: An
Introduction to Linguistic Morphology, Oxford: Oxford University Press
Stump,
Gregory T, 2001, Inflectional Morphology,
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Shibatani,
Masayoshi, 1987. "Japanese". In Bernard Comrie. The World's Major
Languages. Oxford University Press.
Balthasar Bickel &
Johanna Nichols, Inflectional morphology,
University of California, Berkeley
The concise Oxford
dictionary
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar